Why Do We Sleep?

But What About the Platypus?
Health and medicine explained.
March 27 2007 7:26 AM

Why Do We Sleep?

VIEW ALL ENTRIES

This is the second installment of a five-part series.

Click here to launch a slide show
Amanda Schaffer Amanda Schaffer

Amanda Schaffer is a science and medical columnist for Slate.

In the past several years, researchers like Harvard's Robert Stickgold have made the case that sleep plays a critical role in boosting memory and learning. Many scientists believe that sleep must serve some crucial purpose, since sleeping animals can't do useful things like search for food and may be easier targets for hungry predators. Memory processing has been held up as the critical task that might have made sleep worth the risk.

For a dose of concentrated skepticism, I called Jerry Siegel, a maverick professor of psychiatry at the University of California at Los Angeles. He has been jousting with Stickgold since 2001, when he wrote a bruising takedown in Science of the evidence that memory consolidation depends on REM sleep.

"The literature's all over the place in terms of what stage of sleep and what type of memory" is supposedly affected, says Siegel. "The evidence isn't converging. It's contradictory."

Will sleep turn out to play some role in boosting memory? I ask.

"Maybe, but the evidence isn't there. And I doubt it will be an essential role," he says.

Siegel has turned his attention to early egg-laying mammals, like the platypus and echidna (otherwise known as the spiny anteater). He has probed the relationship between these and other animals' snoozing habits and their ecological niches to theorize about the ancient origins of sleep and its evolutionary progression. Not surprisingly, he ends up telling a different story than the memory camp about why we spend up to a third of our lives in slumber.

Human sleep is divided into stages of non-REM and REM, which alternate in a repeating cycle throughout the night. During non-REM sleep, the brain's activity is highly synchronized, with large groups of neurons firing simultaneously. "Imagine a stadium full of people, all quiet, then all shouting at the top of their lungs together, then quiet, then shouting," one sleep researcher told me. During REM, on the other hand, groups of neurons fire at many different frequencies, and brain activity overall resembles that during waking. Also, blood pressure rises, the body's muscles are paralyzed (except for those that control breathing and darting eyes), and men typically get erections (leading another researcher to quip that these are the antennae for catching dreams).

What original functions might non-REM and REM sleep have served? Researchers have long speculated that non-REM plays a housekeeping role, removing a toxin or replenishing a substance depleted during waking. Siegel and his colleagues observed that sleep-deprived rats show damage to some brain-cell membranes, damage that sleep may serve to prevent or repair. As for REM sleep, researchers once believed that it was a recent evolutionary development—absent, for instance, in early egg-laying mammals like the platypus and echidna. In the late 1990s, however, Siegel and colleagues in Australia demonstrated that these throwback animals do experience a form of REM sleep, although only in their brainstems. Siegel argues that REM's original function may have been to stimulate the brainstem, perhaps to warm the brain physically or to ready it for waking.

Siegel also argues that the sleep habits of different animals evolved to help them adapt to their ecological niches. The way they slept varied according to what dangers they faced, when they needed to hunt for food, what kinds of shelter were available to them. In contrast to the usual evolutionary assumptions, he suggests that sleep is not dangerous or maladaptive at all. Rather, it provides a period of safety for animals—keeping them immobile, tucked away in a cave or a burrow, hidden from predators, and less likely to be injured or get into fights.

Consider that different animals have different time slots during the day for productive foraging. Bats, for instance, come out at dusk, when the flies they eat are abundant. "The rest of the time, bats are better off sleeping," Siegel says. "Their reproductive success is going to be diminished if they are active at any other time of the day." Newborn dolphins, on the other hand, do not appear to sleep for more than a minute at a time and may not sleep at all (in contrast to the heavy slumber of most mammalian babes). Siegel suggests that this pattern emerged because dolphins cannot retreat to a safe spot while they're learning to survive in the open ocean. He also connects how much a species sleeps to how time-intensive its foraging needs are. Carnivores, which eat periodic high-calorie meals, tend to sleep more than herbivores, which must spend more time grazing. (Omnivores like us fall somewhere in between.)

Viewed through this lens, memory consolidation does not look like the key function of sleep. Siegel notes that animals can take care of all manner of physiological business (besides eating and having sex) as they while the hours away in slumber. They can secrete hormones, repair cells, or shore up their immune systems. Humans, for one, may engage in various kinds of emotional processing; sleep may "knit up the raveled sleeve of care," as Shakespeare put it. We and other species may consolidate memories. But for Siegel, the variation—and especially the unusual ability of some marine mammals to do without consolidated sleep for periods of time—calls into question the essential nature of any one of sleep's functions.

TODAY IN SLATE

Medical Examiner

The Most Terrifying Thing About Ebola 

The disease threatens humanity by preying on humanity.

I Bought the Huge iPhone. I’m Already Thinking of Returning It.

Scotland Is Just the Beginning. Expect More Political Earthquakes in Europe.

Students Aren’t Going to College Football Games as Much Anymore

And schools are getting worried.

Two Damn Good, Very Different Movies About Soldiers Returning From War

The XX Factor

Lifetime Didn’t Think the Steubenville Rape Case Was Dramatic Enough

So they added a little self-immolation.

Politics

Blacks Don’t Have a Corporal Punishment Problem

Americans do. But when blacks exhibit the same behaviors as others, it becomes part of a greater black pathology. 

Why a Sketch of Chelsea Manning Is Stirring Up Controversy

How Worried Should Poland, the Baltic States, and Georgia Be About a Russian Invasion?

Trending News Channel
Sept. 19 2014 1:11 PM Watch Flashes of Lightning Created in a Lab  
  News & Politics
Weigel
Sept. 20 2014 11:13 AM -30-
  Business
Business Insider
Sept. 20 2014 6:30 AM The Man Making Bill Gates Richer
  Life
Quora
Sept. 20 2014 7:27 AM How Do Plants Grow Aboard the International Space Station?
  Double X
The XX Factor
Sept. 19 2014 11:33 AM Planned Parenthood Is About to Make It a Lot Easier to Get Birth Control
  Slate Plus
Slate Picks
Sept. 19 2014 12:00 PM What Happened at Slate This Week? The Slatest editor tells us to read well-informed skepticism, media criticism, and more.
  Arts
Brow Beat
Sept. 20 2014 3:21 PM “The More You Know (About Black People)” Uses Very Funny PSAs to Condemn Black Stereotypes
  Technology
Future Tense
Sept. 19 2014 5:03 PM White House Chief Information Officer Will Run U.S. Ebola Response
  Health & Science
Bad Astronomy
Sept. 20 2014 7:00 AM The Shaggy Sun
  Sports
Sports Nut
Sept. 18 2014 11:42 AM Grandmaster Clash One of the most amazing feats in chess history just happened, and no one noticed.