Too True To Be Good

How the dismal science applies to your life.
Sept. 16 1999 3:30 AM

Too True To Be Good

The real reason you can't believe everything you read.

34000_34948_neubecker_talkballoon

Not everything you read on the World Wide Web is true. Not everything you read in the New York Times is true, either. So when you read about scientific breakthroughs, how do you know what to believe?

Advertisement

Partly, you trust your instincts: A theory that life evolved from clay is more inherently plausible than a theory that life evolved from Play-Doh. Partly, you consider the source: A Harvard professor is more credible than a Dartmouth dishwasher. And partly you rely on expert judgments: If a prestigious journal has agreed to publish the clay theory, it's probably wrong.

Yes, I meant to say that: If a prestigious journal publishes a theory, it's probably wrong. Given two equally plausible theories from equally credible sources that have passed equally strict scrutiny, the one that makes it into a top journal has a smaller chance of being right. Here's why: Editors like to publish theories they find surprising. And the best way to surprise an editor is to be wrong.

That's not to say that editors are reckless. At least in mathematics and economics (the two fields where I can testify from personal experience), the editorial process is rigorously demanding. Long before an article is submitted for publication, the author is expected to circulate drafts among experts in the field and to respond to their criticisms and comments--a process that typically takes years. Only then is the (now heavily revised) article formally submitted, whereupon the editor handpicks an expert referee to examine it line by line--a process that can easily take another year or more. Are referees ever lax and careless? Surely. Are they lax and careless with articles of genuine importance? In my observation, essentially never. Through multiple rounds of correspondence, referees demand satisfaction regarding every important detail. In many cases, the author will visit the referee's home institution for a semester or a year to be available for periodic grilling.

That's exactly what's so damning about the hoax perpetrated in 1996 by Alan Sokal. Sokal's paper, intentionally stripped of logic, evidence, and even meaning, was accepted for publication in the cultural studies journal Social Text. True, this was a one-time event, but it was an event so far removed from anything that could possibly occur in a legitimate academic enterprise that it converted agnostics like me, who had doubted the status of cultural studies as an intellectual discipline, into hard-core cynics with no doubt whatsoever.

In a serious economics journal, it would be impossible to publish an article like Sokal's. But it would not be impossible, or even unusual, to publish a carefully reasoned article that's still wrong. That's because of the bias I mentioned earlier: Given two papers that have both survived the vetting process, editors tend to prefer the more surprising, which means that on average they prefer the one that's wrong.

Illustration by Robert Neubecker

It's easy to see how the same dynamic could work at a newspaper. "Man bites dog" is a better story than "dog bites man," but it's also more likely to be wrong, even if both stories are reported by equally reliable witnesses. In general--and this observation is a mainstay of college statistics courses--when you think you've seen something unusual, you're more likely to be mistaken than when you think you've seen something ordinary. But it's the unusual that makes the front page.

A few years ago, economics professors J. Bradford De Long and Kevin Lang devised an ingenious way to determine just how many published economic hypotheses are actually true. They looked through several years' worth of issues of the top economics journals and found 78 hypotheses that were confirmed by strong evidence--the sort of evidence that led the authors to accept their own hypotheses. (Another 198 hypotheses were rejected by their authors.) In exactly none of the 78 cases could the confirmation be called overwhelming.

But that's OK. Strong evidence is, after all, strong evidence, even when it's a little shy of overwhelming. In most cases, overwhelming evidence is too much to ask for, because evidence can be hard to collect and hard to interpret. So no individual article can be criticized for failing to live up to an unattainable standard.

But, said De Long and Lang, out of 78 true hypotheses, surely there should be at least a few that are overwhelmingly confirmed. In fact, they gave a precise definition of the word overwhelming, according to which roughly 10 percent of all true hypotheses should come packaged with overwhelming evidence. So if all 78 hypotheses are true, then roughly 7.8 of them--call it eight-- should be confirmed overwhelmingly. And they're not.

TODAY IN SLATE

Politics

The Democrats’ War at Home

How can the president’s party defend itself from the president’s foreign policy blunders?

Congress’ Public Shaming of the Secret Service Was Political Grandstanding at Its Best

Michigan’s Tradition of Football “Toughness” Needs to Go—Starting With Coach Hoke

A Plentiful, Renewable Resource That America Keeps Overlooking

Animal manure.

Windows 8 Was So Bad That Microsoft Will Skip Straight to Windows 10

Politics

Cringing. Ducking. Mumbling.

How GOP candidates react whenever someone brings up reproductive rights or gay marriage.

Building a Better Workplace

You Deserve a Pre-cation

The smartest job perk you’ve never heard of.

Hasbro Is Cracking Down on Scrabble Players Who Turn Its Official Word List Into Popular Apps

Florida State’s New President Is Underqualified and Mistrusted. He Just Might Save the University.

  News & Politics
Jurisprudence
Sept. 30 2014 2:36 PM This Court Erred The Supreme Court has almost always sided with the wealthy, the privileged, and the powerful, a new book argues.
  Business
Moneybox
Sept. 30 2014 7:02 PM At Long Last, eBay Sets PayPal Free
  Life
Lexicon Valley
Sept. 30 2014 1:23 PM What Can Linguistics Tell Us About Writing Better? An Interview with Steven Pinker.
  Double X
The XX Factor
Sept. 30 2014 12:34 PM Parents, Get Your Teenage Daughters the IUD
  Slate Plus
Behind the Scenes
Sept. 30 2014 3:21 PM Meet Jordan Weissmann Five questions with Slate’s senior business and economics correspondent.
  Arts
Brow Beat
Sept. 30 2014 8:54 PM Bette Davis Talks Gender Roles in a Delightful, Animated Interview From 1963
  Technology
Future Tense
Sept. 30 2014 7:00 PM There’s Going to Be a Live-Action Tetris Movie for Some Reason
  Health & Science
Medical Examiner
Sept. 30 2014 6:44 PM Ebola Was Already Here How the United States contains deadly hemorrhagic fevers.
  Sports
Sports Nut
Sept. 28 2014 8:30 PM NFL Players Die Young. Or Maybe They Live Long Lives. Why it’s so hard to pin down the effects of football on players’ lives.